The Old English phoneme /f/ descended in some cases from Proto-Germanic *f, which became [v] between voiced sounds as described above. But /f/ also had another source. In the middle or at the end of words, Old English /f/ was often derived from Proto-Germanic *[β] (also written *ƀ), a fricative allophone of the phoneme *b. Proto-Germanic *b became Old English /b/ only at the start of a word, after [m], or when geminated. In other contexts, it became Old English /f/, pronounced either as [v] or [f] based on its position (the originally voiced fricative was devoiced before voiceless sounds or in final position):
In contrast, the Old English phonemes /θ/ and /s/ generally descend only from Proto-Germanic voiceless *θ and *s. Proto-Germanic *[ð] (a fricative allophone of *d, sometimes derived by voicing of *θ in the context of Verner's Law) regularly developed in all positions into the Old English stop /d/, as in fæder /ˈfæder/ from Proto-Germanic *fadēr [ˈɸɑðɛːr]. Proto-Germanic *z (which existed only as the Verner's Law counterpart of *s) regularly developed to Old English /r/ (a sound change called rhotacism). As a result, some Old English verbs show alternations between /θ/ [θ~ð] and /d/ or between /s/ [s~z] and /r/, although in others this alternation was leveled, resulting in /θ/ [θ~ð] or /s/ [s~z] throughout.
Examples of Old English verbs that retained inherited /θ/–/d/ or /s/–/r/ alternations:
Examples of Old English verbs that leveled the consonant to only /θ/ or only /s/:
The voiceless velar plosive [k] was typically spelled ⟨c⟩. Velar [k] alternated in some circumstances with the voiceless palatal affricate [tʃ], also spelled ⟨c⟩.
The voiced velar plosive [ɡ] and fricative [ɣ] were both typically spelled ⟨g⟩ and can be analyzed as allophones of the same phoneme. In early Old English, the plosive [ɡ] was used only after /n/, as in singan, or as part of the geminate [ɡɡ], as in frogga (also written frocga). (Geminate [ɡɡ] was uncommon, since West Germanic gemination caused palatalization.) In later Old English (possibly after around 950 or 1000 AD), [ɡ] was also used at the start of a word (or at the start of a morpheme in compound or prefixed words), but in early Old English, [ɣ] is believed to have been used in word-initial position. In both early and late Old English, [ɣ] was used medially after vowels or after consonants other than /n/. The sounds [ɡ] and [ɣ] were mostly in complementary distribution. However, either sound could occur after /n/, since phonetic [nɣ] occurred as the result of syncope in some words such as syngian. The phonemic transcription used in this article ignores such exceptional cases and treats [ɣ] and [ɡ] as allophones of a phoneme /ɣ/. As with ⟨c⟩, the letter ⟨g⟩ in Old English represented not only velar but also palatal consonant sounds: [ɣ] had a palatal counterpart [j] and [ɡ] had a palatal counterpart [dʒ], described in the following section.
The voiceless glottal fricative [h] and voiceless velar fricative [x] were both typically spelled ⟨h⟩ and are generally analyzed as allophones of a single phoneme, which can be analyzed as /x/, at least in early Old English. The glottal allophone [h] was used at the start of a word (or at the start of a morpheme in compound or prefixed words), whereas the velar allophone [x] was used at the end of a syllable (by itself or in combination with another consonant) or as part of the geminate [xx]. Thus, hund ('dog') can be transcribed phonetically as [hund], phonemically as /xund/. This phoneme is often assumed to have had a third allophone, a voiceless palatal fricative [ç], used after front vowels (or possibly only after stressed front vowels). For example, cniht ('boy') /knixt/, may have been phonetically realized as [kniçt]. The use of the sound [ç] in this position is supported by developments in English pronunciation seen from the thirteenth century onward: original /x/ sometimes became /f/ after a back vowel (e.g. rough, tough, trough), but this change is never seen after a front vowel. That is explained if it is assumed that the allophone [x] sometimes became [f], but the allophone [ç] never did so. Lass 1994 considers it uncertain that [ç] was used already in Old English, whereas Hogg 1992 regards it as certain that ⟨h⟩ had developed a palatal version, like other velar consonants.
The consonants /ɣ/ and /x/ are analyzed as separate phonemes in at least the early stages of Old English, because it appears that they originally stood in direct contrast at the start of a word (as in [ɣoːd] gōd 'good' vs. [hoːd] hōd 'hood') or at the end of a word (as in [læ͞ɑɣ] lēag 'lye' vs. [læ͞ɑx] lēah 'clearing, meadow').
However, word-final /ɣ/ and /x/ merged in at least some dialects of Old English, judging by the eventual use of spellings with ⟨h⟩ for words that originally ended with /ɣ/ (as well as some cases of "inverted" spellings with a final ⟨g⟩ for words that originally ended with /x/). Such spellings occur regularly in Late West Saxon and Kentish texts from around 900 onwards, suggesting both sounds had come to be pronounced [x] in this position (compare the devoicing of final /f/). They are not attested in older Kentish charters, and are seen only occasionally in Early West Saxon. Spellings with ⟨h⟩ for original /ɣ/ are comparatively rare in Anglian dialects, with hardly any clear examples in Northumbrian texts.
In the middle of a word, [x] or [h] had been lost early on between voiced sounds. Accordingly, there was no direct contrast between /ɣ/ and /x/ in this position. In the same dialects where final /ɣ/ came to be spelled with ⟨h⟩, there are occasional, less systematic examples of word-medial /ɣ/ being written with ⟨h⟩, such as ⟨fuhlas⟩ for fuglas. Spellings like this have been interpreted as evidence that /ɣ/ could be devoiced to [x] in syllable-final, as well as in word-final position, but they may instead simply be cases where ⟨h⟩ was used analogically to represent voiced [ɣ], based on the interchangeability of the spellings ⟨h⟩ and ⟨g⟩ in word-final position: there are also a few examples of ⟨h⟩ being used in place of medial ⟨g⟩ at the start of a word-medial syllable, such as ⟨dahum, sorhe⟩ for dagum, sorge.
It is possible that at some point after the sound changes described above, medial [ɣ] came to be reanalyzed as an allophone of /x/. In Late West Saxon texts, ⟨g⟩ and ⟨h⟩ were in complementary distribution everywhere except for at the start of a word. Word-initial [ɣ] never merged with [h] (/x/), but the eventual replacement of word-initial [ɣ] with the plosive [ɡ] might have been a consequence of the sound becoming phonemically reanalyzed as /ɡ/ in this position.
A morphological contrast is seen between inflected forms with medial -⟨g⟩- [ɣ], and forms that show contraction of adjacent vowels after the loss of original intervocalic [x] or [h]. These alternate in certain classes of strong verbs as a result of Verner's Law: an example is the strong class 6 infinitive slēan (from Proto-West Germanic *slahan) versus the corresponding plural past form slōgon (from Proto-West Germanic *slōgun).
The inflectional paradigms of some words show alternation between [ɣ], [j], and [x] as a result of devoicing and palatalization:
The palatal consonants [tʃ, dʒ, j, ʃ] were represented in Old English spelling with the same letters as velar consonants or clusters [k, ɡ, ɣ, sk]:
Modern editors may mark the palatal consonants with a dot above the letter: ⟨ċ⟩, ⟨ġ⟩, ⟨sċ⟩. Historically, [tʃ, ʃ, dʒ] developed from [k, sk, ɡ] by palatalization. Some cases of [j] developed from palatalization of [ɣ], while others developed from Proto-Germanic *j. Even though palatalization was originally a regular sound change, later sound changes and borrowings meant that the occurrence of the palatal forms was no longer predictable. Thus, palatal and velar consonants eventually became separate phonemes. But there is some debate about when the contrast became phonemic, as well as about when the palatal counterparts of [k ɡ] evolved to affricates [tʃ dʒ] as opposed to palatal plosives [c ɟ]. The forms ⟨orcgeard⟩ and ⟨feccan⟩, attested around 900 AD as unetymological spellings of original ortġeard and fetian, are commonly interpreted as evidence that palatal ċ had become an affricate [tʃ], as it is assumed that these words underwent a change of [tj] to [tʃ]. However, because palatal ċ and velar c alliterate in English poetry up through at least the late tenth century, Minkova 2014 assumes that they were still allophones of a single phoneme before 1000. Likewise, word-initial palatal ġ and velar g alliterate with each other in early Old English verse (before the latter changed to [ɡ], circa 950 AD), which Minkova 2014 interprets as evidence that [j] and [ɣ] constituted allophones at this point in time, despite the existence of /j/ from Proto-Germanic. Lass 1994 assumes that [j], [ɣ] and [ɡ] were all allophones of a phoneme /ɡ/ at one point during the history of Old English.
Palatalized ⟨sċ⟩, according to Minkova 2014, may have still been pronounced as a cluster [sc] rather than as a unitary consonant [ʃ] in some dialects at the end of Old English.
The distribution of velar and palatal consonants is described below.
Before unstressed vowels, ⟨c g sc⟩ can be palatal or velar depending on etymology. Velar [k ɣ sk] can be found before unstressed back vowels in words such as dīcas, plegode, æscas, whereas palatal [tʃ j ʃ] can be found before unstressed back vowels in words that originally contained an etymological *j or *i after the consonant, such as sēċan, wierġan, wȳsċan from Proto-Germanic *sōkijaną, *wargijaną, *wunskijaną. Velar [k ɣ sk] can be found before an unstressed front vowel in class II weak verbs with an infinitive ending in -ian; e.g. wacian, dagian, āscian. The front vowel /i/ is here derived from umlaut, unrounding, shortening and raising of original -ō-: e.g. Proto-West-Germanic *makōn 'to make' was replaced with *makōjan, which is hypothesized to have developed through *makœ̅jan and *makejan to Old English maci(ġ)an. As seen from these examples, the sounds that etymologically caused palatalization of velar consonants also caused i-umlaut of the vowel in the preceding syllable. However, it is not always possible to predict whether a consonant is velar or palatal from the quality of the preceding vowel; some palatal consonants arose after the vowel ⟨i⟩, which is unchanged by i-umlaut (as in rīċu; contrast strīcan) and for historical reasons, some words developed palatal consonants between two back vowels (as in sċeōġeað /ˈʃoːjɑθ/).
The voiced affricate [dʒ] was found only in restricted contexts: it did not occur at the start of a word, and occurred medially or finally only after a nasal or in contexts where it was (at least originally) geminated. It was therefore in nearly complementary distribution with [j]. However, phonetic [nj] occurred as the result of syncope in some words such as menġu (a syncopated form of meniġu 'many; a multitude'). The transcription in this article ignores such exceptional cases and treats [dʒ] as an allophone of /j/.
In circumstances where the palatal affricates [tʃ] and [dʒ] came to be followed by another consonant due to syncope of an intervening vowel, they were eventually replaced with the corresponding velar plosives, [k] and [ɡ] respectively. (Ringe & Taylor 2014 assume this replacement occurred before the palatalized variants had developed into affricates. Campbell 1959 assumes that such consonants were never affricated, but transcribes them as palatal in Old English.) The affricates do seem to have been used before other consonants in compound words, e.g. in bryċġ-bōt 'bridge-repairing' and seċġ-lēac 'sedge-leek, rush-garlic'.
[ŋ] is an allophone of /n/ occurring before [k] and [ɡ]. Words that have final /ŋ/ in standard Modern English have the cluster [ŋɡ] in Old English.
The exact nature of Old English /r/ is not known. It may have been an alveolar approximant [ɹ], as in most Modern English accents; an alveolar flap [ɾ]; or an alveolar trill [r].
The spellings ⟨hw⟩, ⟨hl⟩, ⟨hn⟩, ⟨hr⟩ probably represented two-phoneme clusters, /xw, xl, xn, xr/, where /x/ was pronounced [h] (its usual allophone in syllable-initial position). In this context, /w, l, n, r/ may have been pronounced as voiceless sonorants [ʍ, l̥, n̥, r̥]. The status of ⟨hw⟩, ⟨hl⟩, ⟨hn⟩, ⟨hr⟩ as clusters rather than unitary segments in Old English phonology is supported by their alliteration in poetry with each other and with prevocalic [h] /x/. In addition, variation is seen between the spellings hræn and hærn 'wave, sea', which suggests the ⟨hr⟩ in the first form was not a single consonant phoneme.
There is an alternative hypothesis that holds that (at least in later periods) ⟨h⟩ in these sequences was not pronounced as an independent consonant sound, but was only a diacritic marking the voicelessness of the following sonorant. Original /xr, xn, xl/ would merge with plain /r n l/ by early Middle English. The merger of /xr/ and /r/ seems to have been completed earliest, by the middle of the eleventh century, based on frequent interchange of the spellings ⟨hr⟩ and ⟨r⟩ in glosses from that time period. The merger of /xl/ and /l/ may have taken somewhat longer to complete. The digraphs ⟨rh⟩, ⟨nh⟩ and ⟨lh⟩ are attested to some extent in Middle English texts; e.g. the 12th-century Ormulum contains ⟨rhof⟩ but also includes forms spelled with simple ⟨r⟩. The Ayenbite of Inwyt (written in 1340 by a Kentish English speaker who was probably born during the thirteenth century) contains spellings with ⟨lh⟩ and ⟨nh⟩ alongside spellings with ⟨l⟩ and ⟨n⟩ in words that had hl, hn in Old English.
At least some of these mergers may have begun earlier. Old English scribes occasionally omitted the letter ⟨h⟩ in words starting with these clusters. A merge of the cluster /xw/ with /w/ is also attested in some historical and many current varieties of English, but has still not been completed, as some present-day speakers distinguish the former as [ʍ]. There is evidence of alliteration between ⟨hw⟩ and ⟨w⟩ in some Old English poems.
/l r/ apparently had velarized allophones [ɫ] and [rˠ] or similar sounds when they were followed by another consonant or were geminated. That is suggested by the vowel shifts of breaking and retraction before /l r/, which could be cases of assimilation to a following velar consonant:
Based on phonotactic constraints on initial clusters, Fisiak 1967 proposed interpreting ⟨wr⟩ and ⟨wl⟩ as digraphs representing the velarized sounds in prevocalic position, in which case the distinction would be phonemic, as exhibited by minimal pairs such as wrīdan [ˈrˠiːdɑn] "to grow" vs. rīdan [ˈriːdɑn] "to ride" or wlītan [ˈɫiːtɑn] "to look" vs. lītan [ˈliːtɑn] "to bend". However, this hypothesis is inconsistent with orthoepic and orthographic evidence from the Early Modern English era, as well as borrowings into and from Welsh, which has [wl] and [wr] as genuine initial clusters. Furthermore, in Old English poetry, ⟨wr⟩ and ⟨wl⟩ can alliterate with each other as well as with ⟨w⟩ followed by a vowel, as in "Wēn' ic þæt gē for wlenco, nalles for wræcsīðum" (Beowulf 338).
Old English had a moderately large vowel system. In stressed syllables both monophthongs and diphthongs had short and long versions, which were clearly distinguished in pronunciation. In unstressed syllables, the number of vowel contrasts was generally reduced. Historically, unstressed vowels could be elided in some circumstances.
Depending on dialect, Old English distinguished five to eight vowel qualities in stressed syllables. Each could appear as a long or a short monophthong. An example of two words distinguished by vowel length is god [god] ('god') versus gōd [goːd] ('good').
The front mid rounded vowel /ø(ː)/ (spelled usually as ⟨oe⟩) existed only in some dialects; in others, it was unrounded and merged with /e(ː)/ ⟨e⟩. This merger is seen for both the long and short versions of the vowel in West Saxon and Kentish by around 900 AD, and was complete in Late West Saxon. In Anglian dialects long /øː/ generally remains rounded, but short /ø/ exhibits variable unrounding.
In Kentish, the vowels /æ(ː)/ and /y(ː)/ also merged into /e(ː)/ sometime around the 9th century, leaving /e(ː)/ and /i(ː)/ as the only front vowels in this dialect.
The long and short versions of each vowel were probably pronounced with the same quality, although some reconstructions assume accompanying qualitative distinctions.
Unstressed syllables displayed fewer vowel contrasts. All unstressed vowels came to be shortened, and many texts only show a clear distinction in this context between three vowels, which can be phonemically transcribed as /ɑ e u/. Even this reduced three-way contrast was lost by Middle English, and the merger of unstressed /ɑ e u/ seems in fact to have occurred before the end of the Old English period. While they were probably still distinct in Early West Saxon as spoken in the late ninth century, their spellings become increasingly confused during the tenth and eleventh centuries; thus, Late West Saxon texts show interchange between endings such as -an, -en, -um.
In texts that show a three-way contrast between unstressed vowels, the letters ⟨i⟩ and ⟨o⟩ in unstressed syllables can be analyzed as contextual variants of the phonemes /e/ and /u/ respectively. In the case of /e/, the variant [i] ⟨i⟩ seems to have been used in words ending in -iġ, -iċ, -isċ, -ing, -iht, -liċ (e.g. hāliġ); or in general, in the environment of a following palatal consonant. In the case of /u/, the quality ⟨u⟩ was normally preserved in the endings -um, -ung, -uc or after an accented syllable containing the /u/ sound (as in duguþ); in other contexts (e.g. hēafod, heofon), ⟨u⟩ was variably interchanged with ⟨o⟩ depending on dialect and time period, with the use of ⟨o⟩ generally increasing over time, although there was a tendency to retain ⟨u⟩ in absolute word-final position.
Unstressed /e/ developed from older /æ/ and /i/, and spellings with unstressed ⟨æ⟩ and ⟨i⟩ can be seen in certain early Old English texts.
All dialects of Old English had diphthongs. Diphthongs were written with digraphs composed of two vowel letters and were pronounced by gliding from one vowel quality to another within a single syllable. The two main spellings used to represent diphthongs were ⟨ea⟩ and ⟨eo⟩.
Diphthongs could be short or long. A short diphthong had the same length as a short single vowel, and a long diphthong had the same length as a long single vowel. As with monophthongs, their length was not systematically marked in Old English manuscripts, but is inferred from other evidence, such as a word's etymological origins or the pronunciation of its descendants. Modern editions conventionally mark long diphthongs with a macron on the first letter: e.g. long ⟨ēa⟩, ⟨ēo⟩ in contrast to short ⟨ea⟩, ⟨eo⟩. In this article, short diphthongs such as ea are transcribed like [æ͝ɑ], and long diphthongs such as ēa are transcribed like [æ͞ɑ].
Some dialects had additional diphthongs:
Thus, the inventory of diphthongs in Late West Saxon was as follows:
The table above displays how Old English vowel digraphs are commonly interpreted, but there are various debates about the pronunciation and phonemic analysis of these spellings.
The phonetic realization of Old English diphthongs is controversial.
During the 20th century, various academic articles disputed the reconstruction of "short diphthongs", arguing that they were actually monophthongs (on the phonetic level, the phonemic level, or both). However, in response to these proposals, further arguments have been made in support of the proposition that short digraphs did in fact represent phonetic diphthongs. Hogg 1992 argues that a contrast between long and short diphthongs is not necessarily phonologically implausible, noting it is attested in some modern languages, such as Scots, where the short diphthong in tide /təid/ contrasts with the long diphthong in tied /taid/. In contrast, Minkova 2014 considers the evidence for the phonemic status of short diphthongs to be unconvincing and prefers to analyze short ⟨ea⟩, ⟨eo⟩ as allophones of /æ, e/, or at most, as semi-contrastive entities that never became completely distinct phonemes from the corresponding short monophthongs.
Assuming vowel digraphs were in fact pronounced as phonetic diphthongs, they may have been the 'falling' type (where the first portion of the diphthong was more prominent, and the second part was a non-syllabic offglide. Alternatively, both components may have been more or less equal in prominence.
The primary feature that distinguished ⟨ea⟩ from ⟨eo⟩ seems to have been the height of the first component of the diphthong: the start of ⟨ea⟩ sounded like ⟨æ⟩ /æ/ whereas the start of ⟨eo⟩ sounded like ⟨e⟩ /e/. The second component of any diphthong (whether original or from breaking) seems to have originally been high back rounded [u] (or [u̯]). Diphthongs seem to have still ended in this quality at the time when i-umlaut occurred. Fulk 2014 assumes the qualities [æu̯ eu̯ iu̯ æːu̯ eːu̯ iːu̯] continued to be used into Old English for ea eo io ēa ēo īo respectively, but acknowledges that their values may have been different in late Old English. Ringe & Taylor 2014 assume that by the 9th century, the second component of ea had become lowered and unrounded (aside from in the minority of regions where the alternative spelling ⟨eo⟩ was used for this diphthong). Both components of [æɑ] are low vowels and both components of [eo] are mid vowels. Lass & Anderson 1975 propose that Old English diphthongs were "height-harmonic", that is, that both parts of any diphthong had the same vowel height (high, mid or low) as a rule. The reconstruction of io as [iu] and early West Saxon ie as [iy] is consistent with this principle of height harmony. However, Ringe & Taylor 2014 do not find height harmony convincing as a general rule, arguing that the later development of ie īe points instead to the value [iə̯ iːə̯]. Hogg 2011 considers the lowering of the second element of diphthongs to be related to the development of unstressed vowel qualities; while acknowledging that the height of the first element affected the outcome of the second, Hogg rejects height harmony as an overarching principle, and supposes that io came to be pronounced [io] in Old English, with [iu] only being its early or archaic value. Some other scholars have reconstructed ⟨ea⟩ and ⟨eo⟩ as ending in an unrounded schwa-like glide in Old English. However, there is evidence that Old English eo io ēo īo had rounded outcomes in some dialects of Middle English.
Another controversy concerns the development of ē̆a from ǣ̆, and of ī̆e from ē̆, in the context of West Saxon palatal diphthongization. It is difficult to explain why [æ e] would become [æɑ iy] after a palatal consonant; accordingly, Lass 1994 rejects the reality of this sound change and considers the digraphs in this context to be merely an orthographic device used to indicate that [æ e] were preceded by a palatal consonant. The mainstream position is that ǣ̆ and ē̆ were genuinely diphthongized in this position; it has been proposed that their initial outcomes were something like [eə̯ iə̯], with [eə̯] subsequently merging with ē̆a [æɑ].
Old English diphthongs have several origins. Long diphthongs developed partly from the Proto-Germanic diphthongs *au, *eu, *iu and partly from Old English vowel shifts. Short diphthongs developed only from Old English vowel shifts. Here are examples of diphthongs inherited from Proto-Germanic:
Three vowel shifts produced diphthongs: breaking, back mutation, and palatal diphthongization. Breaking caused Anglo-Frisian short *æ, *e, *i to develop into the short diphthongs ea, eo, io before /x, w/ or a consonant cluster beginning with /r, l/. Anglo-Frisian long *ǣ, *ī developed into the diphthongs ēa and īo before /x/:
Back mutation changed i, e and sometimes a to io, eo and ea before a back vowel in the next syllable:
Palatal diphthongization changed æ, ǣ, e, ē to the diphthongs ea, ēa, ie, īe respectively after the palatalized consonants ġ, sċ, and ċ:
In addition, the back vowels a, o, u (long or short) could be spelled as ⟨ea⟩, ⟨eo⟩, ⟨eo⟩ respectively after ċ, ġ, or sċ. However, rather than indicating the development of a diphthong, these spellings might have just been a convention for marking palatal consonants before a back vowel, since the modern English descendants of such words do not display the typical evolution of the diphthong ⟨eo⟩ to a front vowel:
Peter Schrijver has theorized that Old English breaking developed from language contact with Celtic languages. He says that two Celtic languages were spoken in Britain, Highland British Celtic, which was phonologically influenced by British Latin and developed into Welsh, Cornish and Breton, and Lowland British Celtic, which was brought to Ireland at the time of the Roman conquest of Britain and became Old Irish. Lowland British Celtic had velarization like Old and Modern Irish, which gives preceding vowels a back offglide. That feature came by language contact to Old English and resulted in backing diphthongs.
Early West Saxon ie, īe developed from i-mutation of ea, ēa or io, īo (at the time of i-mutation, the merger of the latter with eo, ēo seems to have not yet occurred). In dialects other than West Saxon, i-mutation instead turned ea, ēa into e, ē and left io, īo unchanged.
As in modern English, there was a distinction in Old English between stressed and unstressed syllables. Stress typically could be found only on the first or leftmost syllable of a root morpheme. In morphologically simple words, this is equivalent to the first syllable of the word: e.g. yfel 'evil', pronounced [ˈyvel]. Non-initial syllables within a morpheme were unstressed.
Inflectional suffixes are inferred to have been fully unstressed, based on the absence of alliteration involving these syllables (although in words with multiple unstressed syllables in a row, such as fremedon [ˈfremedon] 'they did', it is possible that there was some kind of alternating rhythm). Fully unstressed syllables did not contain long vowels or diphthongs.
When a simple word was extended by a derivational suffix, or when two roots capable of standing as free words were combined to form a compound, the primary stress fell on the first syllable of the leftmost root. However, there may have been secondary stress in some circumstances on the first syllable of the later element. In Old English verse, the first root of a compound participates in alliteration, whereas the second root of a compound can be involved in alliteration only as a supporting element, if it starts with the same consonant as the first root. Derivational suffixes and the second elements of compound words appear to display a wider range of vowel contrasts than inflectional suffixes: for example, a diphthong can be seen in the second syllable of the word spelled ⟨arleas⟩ 'honorless' derived from the morphemes ār 'honor' and lēas 'devoid of, bereft of' (as a suffix, '-less'). Since vowel length was not written in Old English, it is less clear to what extent long vowels may have been shortened, or conversely, analogically restored, in such derivational suffixes.
When a word started with a prefix, the primary stress could fall either on the first syllable of the prefix, or on the first syllable of the root that followed the prefix. Whether a prefix was stressed or unstressed depended on the identity of the prefix and on the part of speech of the word. The prefixes ġe- and be- were always fully unstressed, and the prefix for- was nearly always unstressed. In contrast, the prefixes and- and ed- always received primary stress. Other prefixes seem to have generally received primary stress in nouns or adjectives, but not in verbs or adverbs. The prefix hund-, used on numerals for the decades 70-120, was unstressed.
Phonotactics is the study of the sequences of phonemes that occur in languages and the sound structures that they form. When describing syllable structure, a capital letter C can be used to represent a consonant sound and a capital letter V can be used to represent a vowel sound, so a syllable such as 'be' is described as having CV structure (one consonant followed by one vowel). The IPA symbol that shows a division between syllables is the dot [.].
Old English stressed syllables were structured as (C)3V(C)4; that is, one vowel as the nucleus with zero to three consonant phonemes in the onset and zero to four consonant phonemes in the coda. An example of a stressed syllable with the minimal number of phonemes would be ǣ 'law, statute', whereas an example of a stressed syllable with nearly the maximum number of phonemes would be bringst (the syncopated second-person singular present form of the strong verb bringan 'bring').
Onset clusters typically consist of a obstruent /p, b, t, d, k, ɣ, s, ʃ, f, θ, x/ followed by a sonorant /m, n, r, l, w/, although /s/ is allowed as a third element before voiceless stops, and /w/ is allowed before /r, l/. The consonants /j, tʃ/ occur only on their own. (If /n̥, r̥, l̥, ʍ, rˠ, ɫ/ are accepted as their own phonemes, the same can be said of these consonants and of /x/, but these are normally analyzed respectively as /xn, xr, xl, xw, wr, wl/.) Some have proposed analyzing clusters of /s/ and a voiceless stop as single segments. In Old English alliterative poetry, a word-initial sequence of /s/ + voiceless stop alliterates only with itself (with or without a following liquid): that is, ⟨st⟩ and ⟨str⟩ count as a match, as do ⟨sp⟩ and ⟨spl⟩, but ⟨st⟩ and /sp/ do not alliterate with each other.
Unpalatalized /sk/ and /skr/ did not occur as a rule at the start of a word, since in inherited vocabulary, original */sk/ came to be palatalized in this position regardless of what sound followed it. The cluster /sk/ could be found word-medially before a back vowel, e.g. in the words þerscan and discas, although the lack of palatalization in such forms might imply that the /s/ was shared between the first and second syllable. The cluster /skr/ probably occurred medially in malscrung, judging by the forms of the related Middle English malskren and Modern English masker. Kuhn 1970 assumes that /sk/ was found at the start of the word scolere, from Latin scholārius, but Campbell 1959 transcribes it as sċolere; the form sċrift from Latin scrīpt- shows that palatalized sċ- could come to be used at the start of Old English words taken from Latin.
The onset was optional, so syllables could start with a vowel phoneme. In Old English poetry, stressed syllables starting with vowel phonemes all alliterate with each other (regardless of whether the vowels are the same or different). A glottal stop consonant [ʔ] may have been phonetically inserted in this position. (Hogg 2011 views alliteration as inconclusive evidence for initial [ʔ].)
The syllable nucleus was always a vowel in stressed syllables. Stressed monosyllabic words always ended in either a consonant or a long vowel (whether a long monophthong or long diphthong): this can be stated in terms of stressed words having at least two moras of length. In words of two or more syllables, it was possible for the stressed syllable to end in a short vowel (called a light syllable), although two-syllable words more often had a heavy first syllable (one that ended in a consonant or long vowel).
It is possible that certain sonorant consonants, such as /n/ or /l/, could serve as the nucleus of an unstressed syllable. However, it is difficult to determine whether or in which contexts consonants were syllabic in Old English, because the relevant forms show variable spelling (a vowel letter, presumably representing an epenthetic vowel sound, could often be inserted before the sonorant) and variable behavior in verse.
In general, Old English permitted similar kinds of clusters of coda consonants as modern English. In morphologically simple words, most coda clusters started with a sonorant or /s/.
Long (geminate) consonants seem to have become simplified to single consonants when not between vowels. However, [dʒ] (analyzed above as long /jj/) did not merge with single /j/ in this context, but remained a distinct coda.
The following tables show some examples of coda clusters that could occur in Old English, while not necessarily constituting an exhaustive list. Although /j/ might be categorized as a resonant, it had non-resonant allophones, and so will be listed alongside obstruent consonants in the tables below.
Some codas with an obstruent preceded by more than one resonant are attested, often as the result of syncope, e.g:
The following additional two-obstruent coda clusters may rarely occur:
Additional possible three-obstruent clusters include:
Although resonant consonants such as /n/ or /l/ could occur word-finally after another consonant, there is some uncertainty about whether they were pronounced as coda consonants or as syllabic consonants (forming unstressed syllable nuclei) in this context. The tables below show word-final sequences ending in a resonant consonant:
†It is assumed that geminate consonants such as /ll/, /nn/, /mm/ were simplified by the Old English period to single consonants when entirely in a syllable coda.
‡The final /l/ in words ending in /nl/, /ml/, /wl/ could potentially become syllabic or have an epenthetic vowel inserted before it; see below. This possibly could apply also to the final /n/ in /ln/.
Because of the loss of certain vowels in final syllables, Proto-West-Germanic came to have words ending in sequences of an obstruent consonant followed by a resonant consonant: for example, Proto-Germanic *xlaxtraz developed to Proto-West-Germanic *xlaxtr. In the past, it was sometimes assumed that a resonant consonant in such a position must necessarily be syllabic. This assumption is false: there are languages where a syllable can end in an obstruent followed by a resonant, as demonstrated by modern Icelandic, where vatn, býsn, segl, gísl are all monosyllables. There is evidence that this type of coda cluster eventually became disallowed in Old English, because many such words show a spelling with a vowel letter inserted before the consonant, such as hleahtor. However, some words could be spelled with or without an inserted vowel letter in Old English, raising the question of whether there was also variation between different pronunciations. Based on the treatment of such words in poetry, Fulk 1989 argues that their pronunciation changed either during or shortly before the time period when Old English literature was written: when not etymologically preceded by a vowel, resonant consonants in this position were generally nonsyllabic in early Old English verse, whereas in late Old English verse, they came to be syllabic (or preceded by an epenthetic vowel). Fulk finds that the syllabic pronunciations are generally used consistently in poetry from the ninth century or later. The development of a syllabic pronunciation seems to have been affected by the identity of the resonant, the identity of the consonant preceding the resonant, and the weight of the syllable.
Like Frisian, Old English underwent palatalization of the velar consonants /k ɣ/ and fronting of the open vowel /ɑ ɑː/ to /æ æː/ in certain cases. Old English also underwent vowel shifts that were not shared with Old Frisian: smoothing, diphthong height harmonization and breaking. Diphthong height harmonization and breaking resulted in the unique Old English diphthongs io, ie, eo, ea.
Palatalization yielded some Modern English word pairs in which one word has a velar and the other has a palatal or postalveolar. Some of these were inherited from Old English (drink and drench, day and dawn), and others have an unpalatalized form loaned from Old Norse (skirt and shirt).
Old English had four major dialect groups: Kentish, West Saxon, Mercian and Northumbrian. Kentish and West Saxon were the dialects spoken south of a line approximately following the course of the River Thames: Kentish in the easternmost portion of that area and West Saxon everywhere else. Mercian was spoken in the middle part of England and was separated from the southern dialects by the Thames and from Northumbrian by the River Humber. Mercian and Northumbrian are often grouped together as "Anglian".
Modern English descends mostly from the Anglian dialect, rather than the standard West Saxon dialect of Old English. However, since London sits on the Thames, near the boundary of the Anglian, West Saxon and Kentish dialects, some West Saxon and Kentish forms have entered Modern English. For example, the spelling of the verb bury is derived from West Saxon, but the pronunciation /ˈbɛri/ is derived from Kentish.
The largest dialectal differences in Old English occurred between West Saxon and the other groups and occurred mostly in the front vowels, particularly the diphthongs. In Kentish, the vowels æ, e, y would eventually all merge as e (long and short). The primary differences between dialects were the following:
All dialects of Old English seem to have shared palatalization as a sound change, including Northumbrian. Forms in Modern English with hard /k/ and /ɡ/ in which a palatalized sound would be expected from Old English appear to be influenced by Scandinavian.
The prologue to Beowulf:
The Lord's Prayer:
Line | Original | IPA | Translation |
---|---|---|---|
[1] | Fæder ūre þū þe eart on heofonum, | [ˈfæ.der ˈuː.re θuː θe æ͝ɑrt on ˈhe͝o.vo.num] | Our father, you who are in heaven, |
[2] | Sīe þīn nama ġehālgod. | [si͞y θiːn ˈnɒ.mɑ jeˈhɑːɫ.ɣod] | May your name be hallowed. |
[3] | Tōbecume þīn rīċe, | [ˌtoː.beˈku.me θiːn ˈriː.tʃe] | May your kingdom come, |
[4] | Ġeweorðe þīn willa, on eorðan swā swā on heofonum. | [jeˈwe͝orˠ.ðe θiːn ˈwil.lɑ on ˈe͝orˠ.ðan swɑː swɑː on ˈhe͝o.vo.num] | Your will be done, on Earth as in heaven. |
[5] | Ūrne dæġhwamlīcan hlāf sele ūs tōdæġ, | [ˈuːrˠ.ne ˈdæj.hʍɑmˌliː.kɑn hl̥ɑːf ˈse.le uːs toːˈdæj] | Give us our daily bread today, |
[6] | And forġief ūs ūre gyltas, swā swā wē forġiefaþ ūrum gyltendum. | [ɒnd forˠˈji͝yf uːs ˈuː.re ˈɣyl.tɑs swɑː swɑː weː forˠˈji͝y.vɑθ uː.rum ˈɣyl.ten.dum] | And forgive us our debts, as we forgive our debtors. |
[7] | And ne ġelǣd þū ūs on costnunge, ac ālīes ūs of yfele. | [ɒnd ne jeˈlæːd θuː uːs on ˈkost.nuŋ.ɡe ɑk ɑːˈli͞ys uːs of ˈy.ve.le] | And do not lead us into temptation, but rescue us from evil. |
[8] | Sōðlīċe. | [ˈsoːðˌliː.tʃe] | Amen. |